Monday, October 27, 2014

YADDA: There when you need them

This past week I gave a talk "Normal Distributions: Photographic Confusions" at the University of Maryland. On the way to the lecture room I pointed out this bell-shaped distribution of wear on a stairway door. Yet Another Door Distribution Again.

Monday, October 20, 2014

Are You Un-fashionably Late?

Here is a histogram of when people showed up to a party. From FiveThirtyEight, begun by Walt Hickey, but then crowdsourced from readers. Of 803 guest times submitted, the median guest arrived almost an hour after the party's start time. Four guests showed up over 3 hours late. How fashionable is that?
They also looked at a scatterplot of arrival time against number of party guests.  They fit a regression, with only a 5%  R2, and the following interpretation: as host you should expect the mean guest to arrive 42 minutes after the party's start plus 4 minutes for every additional 10 guests. So comparing parties that differed by 10 guests, on average, the guests to the larger party arrived 4 minutes later.

Monday, October 13, 2014

X is for .... oh just forget it!

Journalist David Goldenberg of Five Thirty Eight noted the animals most likely used to represent letters in a sample of 50 children's ABC books from 1820 to 2013. He notes that Zebra was used almost exclusively for the letter "Z". But  note the letter "X". So few words begin with "X" that it was most often totally omitted from alphabet books or as Goldenberg says used by authors, "lamely trotting out a fox or an ox and pointing out its last letter." The modern trend seems to be using scientific words such as Xiphias for swordfish.  

Shown these results, one parent mentioned that Xenops, a genus of ovenbirds, was used in at least two of her son's items (books or toys) and was surprised that "D" for dolphin was not higher in the ranking. But I guess it's hard to top Dog and Duck. And Dr. Susess's ABC Book, for the letter D, dreams up a Duck-Dog!


Monday, October 6, 2014

Happy Birthday Holidays

Like the title asks, "How common is your birthday?" From a decade of data from 1994 to 2004, the shades of color in this display seem to indicate that September is the most common month, and further plots show that September 9, 1999 had the most births. The least common? Holidays. Perhaps the expectant parents themselves and/or health care workers keep the expectant mothers away from delivery on New Years Day, July 4th, Christmas and Christmas Eve, and several days in late November, since Thanksgiving can vary. But Leap Day, February 29th is the surely the least common. Via Visual News, via Redditer UCanDoEat.

This display brings to mind the classical birthday problem and its variations. The classical birthday problem considers the probability that in a set of n people, randomly and independently chosen, that at least one pair have the same birthday. The usual assumption is that birthdays are uniformly distributed throughout the year. The display above shows this not to be the case. Bloom(1973) in the American Mathematical Monthly showed that any non-uniform distribution of birthdays makes sharing more likely. Is is well known that for n=23 people the chances are greater than even of sharing uniformly distributed birthdays. Munford (1977) showed that this value of n=23 is also true for any non-uniform distribution. Berresford (1980) examined this with a non-uniform, data-based, distribution of birthdays, illustrating that the surprising and counter-intuitive and robust value of n=23 yields greater than even odds.




Monday, September 29, 2014

Markov Language

From prooffreader.com  is an interactive color-coded matrix of transition probabilities from any given letter on a row to its following letter in a column. For example, along the row beginning with the letter "h", the darkest hue, represents the highest probability (47.42%), is for the letter that most likely follows, which is "e".
The most likely letter to follow "d" is "-", indicating that the most likely choice is no single letter, but instead "nothing". So that "d" most likely is at the end of a word.

There is a similar graphic of reverse transition probabilities, showing letters that most likely precede a given row choice.
It would be fun to simulate how words would behave when primed with this limited behavior of English. We could use our last post and these graphics of letter transition probabilities to simulate a "Markov language".

Monday, September 22, 2014

Dynamic Visualization of Markov Chains

Here is a visual demonstration of Markov Chains by Victor Powell. We've seen his work before in demonstrating conditional probability. These dynamic views of one, two, and then many state Markov Chains.

The program allows for varying transition probabilities, varying speed of travel between the states, and a realization of the resulting time series of state visits. Another very nice visualization.

Monday, September 15, 2014

Latitude and the Drought

Using California's range of latitude for a color coded plot of the severity of its drought over time, via xkcd.